|
- Instinct
- Motivation
- Normality (behavior)
- Organizational studies
- Radical behaviorism
- Reasoning
- Rebellion
- Social relation
- Theories of political behavior
- Work behavio
DECISION MAKING.
INTRODUCTION.
Sample flowchart representing the decision process to add a new article to Wikipedia.
Decision-making can be regarded as the cognitive process
resulting in the selection of a belief or a course of action among
several alternative possibilities. Every decision-making process
produces a final choice[1]
that may or may not prompt action.Decision making is one of the central
activities of management and is a huge part of any process of
implementation. Good decision making is an essential skill to become an
effective leaders and for a successful career. Decision making is the
study of identifying and choosing alternatives based on the values and
preferences of the decision maker. Making a decision implies that there
are alternative choices to be considered, and in such a case we want not
only to identify as many of these alternatives as possible but to
choose the one that has the highest probability of success or
effectiveness and best fits with our goals, desires, lifestyle, values,
and so on.
Overview
Human performance as regards decisions has been the subject of active research from several perspectives:
- Psychological: examining individual decisions in the context of a set of needs, preferences and values the individual has or seeks.
- Cognitive: the decision-making process regarded as a continuous process integrated in the interaction with the environment.
- Normative: the analysis of individual decisions is concerned with the logic of decision-making and rationality and the invariant choice it leads to.[2]
Decision-making can also be regarded as a problem-solving activity
terminated by a solution deemed to be satisfactory. It is, therefore, a
reasoning or emotional process which can be rational or irrational and can be based on explicit assumptions or tacit assumptions. Most decisions are followed by some form of cost-benefit analysis.[3] Rational choice theory encompasses the notion that people try to maximize benefits while minimizing costs.[4]
Some have argued that most decisions are made unconsciously, if not involuntarily. Jim Nightingale, author of Think Smart – Act Smart,
states that "we simply decide without thinking much about the decision
process." In a controlled environment, such as a classroom, instructors
might try to encourage students to weigh pros and cons before making a
decision. This strategy is known as Franklin's rule.
However, because such a rule requires time, cognitive resources and
full access to relevant information about the decision, this rule may
not best describe how people make decisions.[citation needed]
Logical decision-making is an important part of all science-based professions, where specialists apply their knowledge in a given area to make informed decisions. For example, medical decision-making often involves a diagnosis and the selection of appropriate treatment. Some[which?] research using naturalistic methods shows, however, that in situations with higher time pressure, higher stakes, or increased ambiguities, experts use intuitive decision-making rather than structured approaches – following a recognition primed decision that fits the their experience – and arrive at a course of action without weighing alternatives. Recent robust decision research has formally integrated uncertainty into its decision-making model.[citation needed] Decision analysis recognized and included uncertainties in its theorizing since its conception in 1964.[citation needed]
A major part of decision-making involves the analysis of a finite set
of alternatives described in terms of evaluative criteria. Information overload
occurs when there is a substantial gap between the capacity of
information and the ways in which people may or can adapt. The overload
of information can be related to problem≠ processing and tasking, which
effects decision-making.[5]
These criteria may be benefit or cost in nature. Then the problem might
be to rank these alternatives in terms of how attractive they are to
the decision-maker(s) when all the criteria are considered
simultaneously. Another goal might be to just find the best alternative
or to determine the relative total priority of each alternative (for
instance, if alternatives represent projects competing for funds) when
all the criteria are considered simultaneously. Solving such problems is
the focus of multi-criteria decision analysis
(MCDA), also known as multi-criteria decision-making (MCDM). This area
of decision-making, although very old, has attracted the interest of
many researchers and practitioners and is still highly debated as there
are many MCDA/MCDM methods which may yield very different results when
they are applied on exactly the same data.[6] This leads to the formulation of a decision-making paradox.
In regards to management and decision making, each level of
management is responsible for different things. Top level managers look
at and create strategic plans where the organization's vision, goals,
and values are taken into account to create a plan that is cohesive with
the mission statement. For mid-level managers, tactical plans are
created with specific steps with actions that need to be executed to
meet the strategic objective. Finally, you come to the front-line
managers that are responsible for creating and executing operational
plans. These plans include the policies, processes, and procedures of
the organization. Each must take into account the overall goals and
processes of the organization.
Rational and irrational decision-making
In economics, it is thought that if humans are rational and free to
make their own decisions, then they would behave according to rational
choice theory.[7]
This theory states that people make decisions by determining the
likelihood of a potential outcome, the value of the outcome and then
multiplying the two. For example, with a 50% chance of winning $20 or a
100% chance of winning $10, people more likely to choose the first
option.[7]
In reality, however, there are some factors that affect
decision-making abilities and cause people to make irrational decisions,
one of them being availability bias.
Availability bias is the tendency for some items that are more readily
available in memory to be judged as more frequently occurring.[7]
For example, someone who watches a lot of movies about terrorist
attacks may think the frequency of terrorism to be higher than it
actually is.
Information overload
Information overload is "a gap between the volume of information and the tools we need to assimilate it."[8] It is proven in some studies[which?] that the more information overload, the worse the quality of decisions made. There are five factors:
- Personal Information Factors: personal qualifications, experiences, attitudes etc.
- Information Characteristics: information quality, quantity and frequency
- Tasks and Process: standardized procedures or methods
- Organizational Design: organizations' cooperation, processing capacity and organization relationship
- Information Technology: IT management, and general technology
Hall, Ariss & Todorov with an assistant Rashar phinyor (2007)
described an illusion of knowledge, meaning that as individuals
encounter too much knowledge it actually interferes with their ability
to make rational decisions.[9]
Problem analysis vs. decision-making
It is important to differentiate between problem analysis
and decision-making. The concepts are completely separate from one
another. Traditionally, it is argued that problem analysis must be done
first, so that the information gathered in that process may be used
towards decision-making.[10]
- Problem analysis
- Analyze performance, what should the results be against what they actually are
- Problems are merely deviations from performance standards
- Problem must be precisely identified and described
- Problems are caused by a change from a distinctive feature
- Something can always be used to distinguish between what has and hasn't been affected by a cause
- Causes to problems can be deducted from relevant changes found in analyzing the problem
- Most likely cause to a problem is the one that exactly explains all the facts
- Decision-making
- Objectives must first be established
- Objectives must be classified and placed in order of importance
- Alternative actions must be developed
- The alternative must be evaluated against all the objectives
- The alternative that is able to achieve all the objectives is the tentative decision
- The tentative decision is evaluated for more possible consequences
- The decisive actions are taken, and additional actions are taken to
prevent any adverse consequences from becoming problems and starting
both systems (problem analysis and decision-making) all over again
- There are steps that are generally followed that result in a
decision model that can be used to determine an optimal production plan.[11]
- In a situation featuring conflict, role-playing may be helpful for predicting decisions to be made by involved parties.[12]
Decision planning
Making a decision without planning is fairly common, but does not
often end well. Planning allows for decisions to be made comfortably and
in a smart way. Planning makes decision-making a lot more simple than
it is.
Decision will get four benefits out of planning: 1. Planning give
chance to the establishment of independent goals. It is a conscious and
directed series of choices. 2. Planning provides a standard of
measurement. It is a measurement of whether you are going towards or
further away from your goal. 3. Planning converts values to action. You
think twice about the plan and decide what will help advance your plan
best. 4. Planning allows for limited resources to be committed in an
orderly way. Always govern the use of what is limited to you. (e.g.
money, time, etc.)[13]
Analysis paralysis
Analysis paralysis
is the state of over-analyzing (or over-thinking) a situation, or
citing sources, so that a decision or action is never taken, in effect
paralyzing the outcome.
Everyday techniques
Decision-making techniques can be separated into two broad categories: Group decision-making and individual decision-making techniques.
Group decision-making techniques
- Consensus decision-making
tries to avoid "winners" and "losers". Consensus requires that a
majority approve a given course of action, but that the minority agree
to go along with the course of action. In other words, if the minority
opposes the course of action, consensus requires that the course of
action be modified to remove objectionable features.
- Voting-based methods.
- Range voting
lets each member score one or more of the available options. The option
with the highest average is chosen. This method has experimentally been
shown to produce the lowest Bayesian regret among common voting methods, even when voters are strategic.[citation needed]
- Majority
requires support from more than 50% of the members of the group. Thus,
the bar for action is lower than with unanimity and a group of "losers"
is implicit to this rule.[citation needed]
- Plurality, where the largest block in a group decides, even if it falls short of a majority.
- Delphi method
is structured communication technique for groups, originally developed
for collaborative forecasting but has also been used for policy making.
- Dotmocracy
is a facilitation method that relies on the use of special forms called
Dotmocracy Sheets to allow large groups to collectively brainstorm and
recognize agreement on an unlimited number of ideas they have authored.
Individual decision-making techniques
- Pros and cons: listing the advantages and disadvantages of each option, popularized by Plato and Benjamin Franklin.[14][15] Contrast the costs and benefits of all alternatives. Also called "rational decision-making".
- Simple prioritization: choosing the alternative with the highest probability-weighted utility for each alternative (see Decision analysis).
- Satisficing: examining alternatives only until an acceptable one is found.
- Elimination by aspects: choosing between alternatives using Mathematical psychology[16] The technique was introduced by Amos Tversky
in 1972. It is a covert elimination process that involves comparing all
available alternatives by aspects. The decision-maker chooses an
aspect; any alternatives without that aspect are then eliminated. The
decision-maker repeats this process with as many aspects as needed until
there remains only one alternative[17]
- Preference trees: In 1979, Tversky and Shmuel Sattach updated the
elimination by aspects technique by presenting a more ordered and
structured way of comparing the available alternatives. This technique
compared the alternatives by presenting the aspects in a decided and
sequential order. It became a more hierarchical system in which the
aspects are ordered from general to specific [18]
- Acquiesce to a person in authority or an "expert"; "just following orders".
- Flipism: flipping a coin, cutting a deck of playing cards, and other random or coincidence methods[19]
- Prayer, tarot cards, astrology, augurs, revelation, or other forms of divination.
- Taking the most opposite action compared to the advice of mistrusted authorities (parents, police officers, partners...)
- Opportunity cost: calculating the opportunity cost of each options and decide the decision.
- Bureaucratic: set up criteria for automated decisions.
- Political: negotiate choices among interest groups.
- Participative decision-making
(PDM): a methodology in which a single decision-maker, in order to take
advantage of additional input, opens up the decision-making process to a
group for a collaborative effort.
- Use of a structured decision-making method.[20]
Individual decision-making techniques can often be applied by a group as part of a group decision-making technique.
A need to use software for a decision-making process
is emerging for individuals and businesses. This is due to increasing
decision complexity and an increase in the need to consider additional
stakeholders, categories, elements or other factors that effect
decisions.
Stages of group decision-making
According to B. Aubrey Fisher,[citation needed] there are four stages or phases that should be involved in all group decision-making:
- Orientation. Members meet for the first time and start to get to know each other.
- Conflict. Once group members become familiar with each other,
disputes, little fights and arguments occur. Group members eventually
work it out.
- Emergence. The group begins to clear up vague opinions by talking about them.
- Reinforcement. Members finally make a decision and provide justification for it.
It is said that critical norms in a group improves the quality of
decisions, while the majority of opinions (called consensus norms) do
not. This is due to collaboration between one another, and when group
members get used to, and familiar with, each other, they will tend to
argue and create more of a dispute to agree upon one decision. This does
not mean that all group members fully agree; they may not want argue
further just to be liked by other group members or to "fit in".[21]
Decision-making steps
Each step in the decision-making process may include social,
cognitive and cultural obstacles to successfully negotiating dilemmas.
It has been suggested that becoming more aware of these obstacles allows
one to better anticipate and overcome them.[22] The Arkansas program presents eight stages of moral decision-making based on the work of James Rest:
- Establishing community: creating and nurturing the relationships,
norms, and procedures that will influence how problems are understood
and communicated. This stage takes place prior to and during a moral
dilemma.
- Perception: recognizing that a problem exists.
- Interpretation: identifying competing explanations for the problem, and evaluating the drivers behind those interpretations.
- Judgment: sifting through various possible actions or responses and determining which is more justifiable.
- Motivation: examining the competing commitments which may distract
from a more moral course of action and then prioritizing and committing
to moral values over other personal, institutional or social values.
- Action: following through with action that supports the more
justified decision. Integrity is supported by the ability to overcome
distractions and obstacles, developing implementing skills, and ego
strength.
- Reflection in action.
- Reflection on action.
Other decision-making processes have also been proposed. One such process, proposed by Pam Brown of Singleton Hospital in Swansea, Wales, breaks decision-making down into seven steps:[23]
- Outline your goal and outcome.
- Gather data.
- Develop alternatives (i.e., brainstorming)
- List pros and cons of each alternative.
- Make the decision.
- Immediately take action to implement it.
- Learn from and reflect on the decision.
Cognitive and personal biases
Biases usually creep into decision-making processes. Many different people have made a decision about the same question (e.g.
"Should I have a doctor look at this troubling breast cancer symptom
I've discovered?" "Why did I ignore the evidence that the project was
going over budget?") and then craft potential cognitive interventions
aimed at improving the outcome of decision-making.
Here is a list of commonly-debated biases in judgment and decision-making.
- Selective search for evidence (aka confirmation bias; Scott Plous,
1993). People tend to be willing to gather facts that support certain
conclusions but disregard other facts that support different
conclusions. Individuals who are highly defensive in this manner show
significantly greater left prefrontal cortex activity as measured by EEG than do less defensive individuals.[24]
- Premature termination of search for evidence. People tend to accept the first alternative that looks like it might work.
- Cognitive inertia. Unwillingness to change existing thought patterns in the face of new circumstances.
- Selective perception. We actively screen out information that we do not think is important (see also prejudice).
In one demonstration of this effect, discounting of arguments with
which one disagrees (by judging them as untrue or irrelevant) was
decreased by selective activation of right prefrontal cortex.[25]
- Wishful thinking. A tendency to want to see things in a certain – usually positive – light, which can distort perception and thinking.[26]
- Choice-supportive bias occurs when people distort their memories of chosen and rejected options to make the chosen options seem more attractive.
- Recency. People tend to place more attention on more recent
information and either ignore or forget more distant information (see semantic priming). The opposite effect in the first set of data or other information is termed primacy effect.[27]
- Repetition bias. A willingness to believe what one has been told most often and by the greatest number of different sources.
- Anchoring and adjustment. Decisions are unduly influenced by initial information that shapes our view of subsequent information.
- Group think. Peer pressure to conform to the opinions held by the group.
- Source credibility bias. A tendency to reject a person's statement
on the basis of a bias against the person, organization, or group to
which the person belongs. People preferentially accept statement by
others that they like (see prejudice).
- Incremental decision-making and escalating commitment. We look at a
decision as a small step in a process and this tends to perpetuate a
series of similar decisions. This can be contrasted with "zero-based
decision-making" (see slippery slope).
- Attribution asymmetry.
People tend to attribute their own success to internal factors,
including abilities and talents, but explain their failures in terms of
external factors such as bad luck. The reverse bias is shown when people
explain others' success or failure.
- Role fulfillment. A tendency to conform to others' decision-making expectations.
- Underestimating uncertainty and the illusion of control.
People tend to underestimate future uncertainty because of a tendency
to believe they have more control over events than they really do.
- Framing bias. This is best avoided by using numeracy with absolute measures of efficacy.[28]
- Sunk-cost fallacy.
A specific type of framing effect that affects decision-making. It
involves an individual making a decision about a current situation based
on what they have previously invested in the situation.[29]
A possible example to this would be an individual that is refraining
from dropping a class that that they are most likely to fail, due to the
fact that they feel as though they have done so much work in the course
thus far.
- Prospect theory.
Involves the idea that when faced with a decision-making event, an
individual is more likely to take on a risk when evaluating potential
losses, and are more likely to avoid risks when evaluating potential
gains. This can influence one's decision-making depending if the
situation entails a threat, or opportunity.[30]
Reference class forecasting was developed to eliminate or reduce cognitive biases in decision-making.
Post-decision analysis
Evaluation and analysis of past decisions is complementary to decision-making; see also mental accounting and postmortem documentation.
Cognitive styles
Influence of Myers-Briggs type
According to behavioralist Isabel Briggs Myers, a person's decision-making process depends to a significant degree on their cognitive style.[31] Myers developed a set of four bi-polar dimensions, called the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). The terminal points on these dimensions are: thinking and feeling; extroversion and introversion; judgment and perception; and sensing and intuition.
She claimed that a person's decision-making style correlates well with
how they score on these four dimensions. For example, someone who scored
near the thinking, extroversion, sensing, and judgment ends of the
dimensions would tend to have a logical, analytical, objective,
critical, and empirical decision-making style. However, some[who?] psychologists say that the MBTI lacks reliability and validity and is poorly constructed.
Other studies suggest that these national or cross-cultural differences exist across entire societies. For example, Maris Martinsons has found that American, Japanese and Chinese business leaders each exhibit a distinctive national style of decision-making.[32]
Optimizing vs. satisficing
Herbert A. Simon coined the phrase "bounded rationality"
to express the idea that human decision-making is limited by available
information, available time and the mind's information-processing
ability. Simon also defined two cognitive styles: maximizers try to make an optimal decision, whereas satisficers
simply try to find a solution that is "good enough". Maximizers tend to
take longer making decisions due to the need to maximize performance
across all variables and make tradeoffs carefully; they also tend to
more often regret their decisions (perhaps because they are more able
than satisficers to recognise that a decision turned out to be
sub-optimal).[33]
Combinatorial vs. positional
Styles and methods of decision-making were elaborated by Aron Katsenelinboigen, the founder of predispositioning theory.
In his analysis on styles and methods, Katsenelinboigen referred to the
game of chess, saying that “chess does disclose various methods of
operation, notably the creation of predisposition – methods which may be
applicable to other, more complex systems.”[34]
In his book, Katsenelinboigen states that apart from the methods
(reactive and selective) and sub-methods (randomization,
predispositioning, programming), there are two major styles: positional
and combinational. Both styles are utilized in the game of chess.
According to Katsenelinboigen, the two styles reflect two basic
approaches to the uncertainty:
deterministic (combinational style) and indeterministic (positional
style). Katsenelinboigen’s definition of the two styles are the
following.
The combinational style is characterized by:
- a very narrow, clearly defined, primarily material goal; and
- a program that links the initial position with the final outcome.
In defining the combinational style in chess, Katsenelinboigen writes:
The combinational style features a clearly formulated limited
objective, namely the capture of material (the main constituent element
of a chess position). The objective is implemented via a well-defined,
and in some cases, unique sequence of moves aimed at reaching the set
goal. As a rule, this sequence leaves no options for the opponent.
Finding a combinational objective allows the player to focus all his
energies on efficient execution, that is, the player’s analysis may be
limited to the pieces directly partaking in the combination. This
approach is the crux of the combination and the combinational style of
play.[34]
The positional style is distinguished by:
- a positional goal; and
- a formation of semi-complete linkages between the initial step and final outcome.
“Unlike the combinational player, the positional player is occupied,
first and foremost, with the elaboration of the position that will allow
him to develop in the unknown future. In playing the positional style,
the player must evaluate relational and material parameters as
independent variables. ... The positional style gives the player the
opportunity to develop a position until it becomes pregnant with a
combination. However, the combination is not the final goal of the
positional player—it helps him to achieve the desirable, keeping in mind
a predisposition for the future development. The pyrrhic victory is the best example of one’s inability to think positionally."[35]
The positional style serves to:
- create a predisposition to the future development of the position;
- induce the environment in a certain way;
- absorb an unexpected outcome in one’s favor;
- avoid the negative aspects of unexpected outcomes.
Katsenelinboigen writes:
- "As the game progressed and defense became more sophisticated the
combinational style of play declined. ... The positional style of chess
does not eliminate the combinational one with its attempt to see the
entire program of action in advance. The positional style merely
prepares the transformation to a combination when the latter becomes
feasible.”[36]
Neuroscience
The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), orbitofrontal cortex and the overlapping ventromedial prefrontal cortex are brain regions involved in decision-making processes. A recent neuroimaging study[37]
found distinctive patterns of neural activation in these regions
depending on whether decisions were made on the basis of perceived
personal volition or following directions from someone else. Patients with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex have difficulty making advantageous decisions.[38]
A recent study[39] of a two-alternative forced choice task involving rhesus monkeys found that neurons in the parietal cortex
not only represent the formation of a decision but also signal the
degree of certainty (or "confidence") associated with the decision.
Another recent study[40] found that lesions to the ACC in the macaque
resulted in impaired decision-making in the long run of reinforcement
guided tasks suggesting that the ACC may be involved in evaluating past
reinforcement information and guiding future action.
Emotion appears able to aid the decision-making process. Decision-making often occurs in the face of uncertainty about whether one's choices will lead to benefit or harm (see also risk). The somatic-marker hypothesis is a neurobiological
theory of how decisions are made in the face of uncertain outcome. This
theory holds that such decisions are aided by emotions, in the form of
bodily states, that are elicited during the deliberation of future
consequences and that mark different options for behavior as being
advantageous or disadvantageous. This process involves an interplay
between neural systems that elicit emotional/bodily states and neural
systems that map these emotional/bodily states.[41]
Although it is unclear whether the studies generalize to all
processing, subconscious processes have been implicated in the
initiation of conscious volitional movements. See the Neuroscience of free will.
Decision-making in adolescents vs. adults
During their adolescent years, teens are known for their high-risk
behaviors and rash decisions. There has not, however, been that much
research in this area. Recent research[citation needed]
has shown, though, that there are some differences in cognitive
processes between adolescents and adults during decision-making.
Researchers have concluded that differences in decision-making are not
due to a lack of logic or reasoning, but more due to the immaturity of psychosocial capacities, capacities that influence decision-making. Examples would be impulse control, emotion regulation, delayed gratification and resistance to peer pressure.
In the past, researchers have thought that adolescent behavior was
simply due to incompetency regarding decision-making. Currently,
researchers have concluded that adults and adolescents are both
competent decision-makers, not just adults. However, adolescents’
competent decision-making skills decrease when psychosocial capacities
become present.
Recent research[citation needed]
has shown that risk-taking behaviors in adolescents may be the product
of interactions between the socioemotional brain network and its cognitive-control network. The socioemotional part of the brain processes social and emotional stimuli and has been shown to be important in reward processing.
The cognitive-control network assists in planning and self-regulation.
Both of these sections of the brain change over the course of puberty.
However, the socioemotional network changes quickly and abruptly, while
the cognitive-control network changes more gradually. Because of this
difference in change the cognitive-control network, which usually
regulates the socioemotional network, [the adolescent?] struggles to
control the socioemotional network when psychosocial capacities are
present.[clarification needed]
When adolescents are exposed to social and emotional stimuli, their
socioemotional network is activated as well as areas of the brain
involved in reward processing. Because teens often gain a sense of
reward from risk-taking behaviors, their repetition becomes ever more
probable due to the reward experienced. In this, the process mirrors addiction.
Teens can become addicted to risky behavior because they are in a high
state of arousal and are rewarded for it not only by their own internal
functions but also by their peers around them.
This is why adults are generally better able to control their
risk-taking because their cognitive-control system has matured enough to
the point where it can control the socioemotional network, even in the
context of high arousal or when psychosocial capacities are present.
Also, adults are less likely to find themselves in situations that push
them to do risky things. For example, teens are more likely to be around
peers who peer pressure them into doing things, while adults are not as
exposed to this sort of social setting.[42][43]
See also
|